WHY STUDENTS FORGET WHAT THEY LEARNED IN SCHOOL?


WHY STUDENTS FORGET WHAT THEY LEARNED IN SCHOOL?
It is customary to blame forgetting on a bad memory and there is no doubt that people do differ considerably in their innate capacity to remember things. We forget because we want to do so. One may retort that it is unfortunately just the things one wants to forget which insist on remaining vividly in our memories.
A good instance is in an ancient formula for changing lead into gold. The process is a somewhat complicated one, but the crucial part requires that we run round a churchyard three times at midnight and not once during the period think of the word abracadabra. The process is quite certain, we are assured if we carry out the instructions precisely. Ofcourse abracadabra refuses to remain out of our thoughts and so we fail.
We all have the experience of forgetting to answer letters and usually we must admit that we didn’t want to answer the letter. If a letter is likely to bring a cheque or some specially welcome news in return we seldom forget.
According to Daniel T. Willingham, a professor of Cognitive Psychology at the University of Virginia, suggests that we certainly forget things overtime and there is no reason to expect that what students learn in school should be any exception. But take heart: we don’t forget everything, and under some conditions we remember everything. Researchers have some understanding of why we are likely to overestimate what we have forgotten. And most important, there is some evidence that the memory of what we have learned in school matters and actually makes us smarter.
What is Memory?
According to Feldman a professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of Massachusetts at Amherst, defines Memory as the processes that allows us to record, store and later retrieve experiences and information (Feldman, 2004).
Memory adds richness and context to our lives but even more fundamentally, it allows us to learn from experience and thus adapt to changing environment.
Memory is the retention of information overtime through encoding, storage and retrieval (Morris, 1973).
In sum, Psychologists consider memory as the process by which we encode, store and retrive information. Each of the three parts of this definition- encoding, storage and retrieval represents a different process. You can think of these processes as analogous to the functions of a computer’s keyboard (encoding), disk (storage) and screen/monitor (retrieval).
MODEL
#Encoding: Is the way in which information is processed for storage in memory. In short, encoding is the way in which information gets into memory storage.
When you are listening to a lecture, watching a movie, listening to music, a talking with a friend, you are encoding information into memory. In everyday experiences, encoding has much in common with learning.
Some information gets into memory virtually automatically, whereas getting other information takes effort. Let’s examine some encoding processes that require effort.
The issues that interest psychologists include how effectively we attend to information, how deeply we process it, how extensively we elaborate it with details and how much we use mental imagery to encode it.
#Storage: Is the way in which information is retained overtime and how it is represented in memory.
#Retrieval: Ability to access stored information in memory.
THREE SYSTEMS OF MEMORY STORAGE
The quality of encoding is not the only thing that determines the quality of memory. The memory also needs to be stored properly after it is encoded. Storage encompasses how information is retained overtime and how it is represented in memory.
We remember some information for less than a second, some for half a minute, and some for minutes, hours, years, even a lifetime. Richard Atkinson and Richard Shiffrin (1968) formulated a very influential theory of memory that acknowledged the varying life span of memories.
The Atkinson-Shiffrin theory states that memory storage involves a system characterized by time frames.
Sensory memory: time frames of a fraction of a second to several seconds. Sensory memory holds information from the world in its original form only for an instant, not much longer than the brief time it is exposed  to the visual, auditory and other senses (sense organs).
Short-term memory: time frames up to 30 seconds. Short-term memory is a limited-capacity memory system in which information is retained for only as long as 30 seconds unless strategies are used to retain it longer. A three-part system that temporarily hold information is called working memory.
Working memory is a kind of mental workbench on which information is manipulated and assembled to perform other cognitive tasks.
According to research, the capacity of short-term memory is greater when information is stored not phonologically (by how it sounds) but visually (by how it looks).
Long-term memory: time frames up to a lifetime. Long-term memory is a relatively permanent system of memory that holds huge amount of information for a long period of time.
Types of Long-term Memory
●Semantic memory: Portion of long-term memory that stores general facts and information. Semantic memory is a person’s knowledge about the world. Example; Recalling tha capital of Ohio.
●Episodic memory: Portion of long-term memory that stores specific information that has personal meaning. The retention of information about the where and when of life’s happenings. Example; Recalling where you went on your first date.
●Implicit memory: Memory for information that either was not intentionally committed to memory or that is retrieved unintentionally from memory. Example; Suddenly remembering that today is a friend’s birthday.
●Explicit memory: Memory for information that is intentionally committed to memory and intentionally retrieved from memory. The conscious recollection of information such as specific facts or events and atleast in humans, information that can be verbally communicated. Example; Recalling the definition of “slave trade” during an exam.
The contention that we forget most of our education is wrong. Under some conditions, we remember nearly everything
Nevertheless, the contention that we forget most of our education is wrong. Naturally, lessons learned in school are subject to forgetting like any other experience but some of what we learned stays with us.
What is Forgetting?
Is the inability to recall or recognise something that has previousily been learned (Passer, 2008).
This may be due either to a lack of availability, as in the case of decay when the information has disappeared, or to a lack of accessibility, as in the case of cue-dependent forgetting when the memory is stored somewhere but can’t be found at that time.
Reasons to Why Students Forget What They Learned in School
#1: Encoding Failure
This is the most cause of forgetting. The information never actually made it to the person’s memory bank. This happen when a student fails to focus on what is being taught.
May be they had other things on their mind that day, or the material just was not engaging enough to capture thier attention.
Another reason for not paying attention is that the student did not see a reason for learning this information; it did not see to have a purpose related to the subject at hand.
This encoding failure can also occur when someone is being presented with too much information all at once, and using them to have to pick and choose what the brain will retain.
#2: Interference
According to interference theory states that people forget not because memories are actually lost from storage but because other information gets in the of what we want to remember.
It is known that recent things we learn are more easily remembered than older information. If both sets of information on the same subject, the newer memories may make it harder, if not impossible to remember the older ones on the same subject.
There are two types of interference which are Proactive interference and Retroactive interference.
Proactive interference is the process by which old material already in memory interferes with new information.
Suppose that Mwl. Juma changes residences, gets a new phone number and meorizes it. That night, when a friend asks Mwl. Juma for her new phone number, he can recall only three digits and instead keeps remembering his old phone number. Memory of his old phone number is interfering with his ability to retrieve the new one.
Retroactive interference is the process by which new information interferes with old material already in memory.
Suppose Anna has now had her new phone number for two months and recalls it perfectly. If we ask her, “What was your old number?” Anna may have trouble recalling it, perhaps mixing up the digits with her new number. In general, the more simiral two sets of information are, the more likely it is that interference will occur.
Why does interference occur? It takes time for the brain to convert short-term memories into long-term memories, and some researchers propose that when new information is entered into the system it can disrupt (i.e retroactively interference with) the conversion of older information into long-term memories (Wixted, 2005).
Others believe that once long-term memories are formed, retroactive and proactive interference are caused by competition among retrieval cues (Anderson & Neely, 1996). When different memories become associated with similar or identical retrieval cues confusion can result and accessing a cue may call up the wrong memory.
Retrieval failure also can occur because we have too few retrieval cues or the cues may be too weak.
#3: Shallow Processing
Deep processing occurs when meaning is placed on the material being learned. When a student can find a reason to retain the information, when an emotion is connected to the material or when more than one sense is used in learning, the memory gains staying power and is more easily brought forth when needed.
If the material can’t be connected to a sense or a feeling, the processing is shallow and it does not stay in the brain for any length of time because the brain considers it unimportant and will make room for things it considers more relevant.
#4: What Versus How
#5: Decay or Disuse
According to decay theory states that when something new is learned, a neurochemical memory trace is formed, but overtime this trace tends to disintegrate.
#6: Failure to rehearse information
According to rehearsal means repetition of information that has entered short-term memory.
There are two types of rehearsing the information which are Rote rehearsal and Elaborative rehearsal.
Rote rehearsal: retaining information in short-term memory simply by repeating it over and over.
Elaborative rehearsal: The linking of new information in short-term memory to familiar material stored in long-term memory.
The transfer of material from short-term memory to long-term memory proceeds largely on the basis of rehearsal, the repetition of information that has entered short-term memory. Rehearsal accomplishes two things. First, as long as the information is repeated, it is maintained in short-term memory. More important, however, rehearsal allows us to transfer the information into long-term memory. If the stored information in memory is not rehearsed over and over, it will be easy to forget.
#7: Amnesia
The term amnesia commonly refers to memory loss due to special conditions such as brain injury, illness, or psychological trauma.
There are two types of amnesia which are Retrograde amnesia and Anterograde amnesia.
Retrograde amnesia: represents memory loss for events that took place sometime in life before the onset of amnesia. Example; Brain operation may cause a person to loss memory. Also, football players experience retrograde amnesia when they are knocked out by a concussion, regain conciousness and cannot remember the events just before being hit.
Anterograde amnesia: refers to memory loss for events that occur after the initial onset of amnesia.
#8: Dementia and Alzheimer’s Didease
Dementia refers to impaired memory and other cognitive deficits that accompany brain degeneration and interfere with normal functioning. There are more than a dozen types and causes of dementia, and although it can occur at any point of life. Dementia is most prevalent among elderly adults.
Alzheimer’s disease is a progressive brain disorder that is the most common cause of dementia among adults over the age of 65.
Estimates suggest that in the year 2000, 4.5 million Americans had Alzheimer disease. By 2050, the number of cases is predicted to rise to about 13 million (Herbert et al., 2003).
The early symptoms of Alzheimer disease which worsen gradually over a period of years, include forgetfulness, poor judgement, confusion and disorientation. Often, memory for recent events and new information is especially impaired.
Alzheimer disease spreads across the temporal lobes and to the frontal lobes and other cortical regions of the brain.
#9: Infantile (Childhood) Amnesia
There is one type of amnesia that almost all of us encounter an inability to remember personal experiences from the first few years of our lives.
Even though infants and preschoolers can form long-term memories of events in their lives. As adults we typically are unable to recall these events consciously. The memory loss for early experiences is called infantile amnesia or childhood amnesia.
Eacott and Crawley (1998) proposed that, our memories of childhood typically do not include events that occured before the age of 3 0r 4, although some adults can partially recall major events. For example; the birth of a sibling, hospitalization, or a death in the family) that happened before the age of 2.
Infantile amnesia is caused by the immature of brain in the first years after birth. Also, we do not encode our earliest experiences deeply and fail to form rich retrieval cues for them. Additionally, because infants lack a clear self-concept, they do not have a personal frame of reference around which to organize rich memories.
Knowledge Retention Strategies for Helping Students’ Remembering of What They Learned in School
1.Assign students frequent practice tests or quizzes
When students are given tests or quizzes that they are not graded on, they are able to review material in a low-stress environment (stress can undermine memory retention).
2.Combine visual and verbal lessons
Learning to use multiple senses helps increase retention. Showing students visual aids while teaching a lesson verbally helps to illustrate and cement the message for students.
3.Encourage and help students to develop memory cues
Examples include acronyms like “Roy G.. Biv” (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, violet) to remember the colors of the rainbow and “Pretty please my dear aunt sally” (parentheses, powers, multiplication, division, addition, subtraction) to remember mathematics’ order of operation.
Another way to “cue” memories is to create memory- boosting songs about lessons.
4.Encourage peer discussion and group-based learning
Group discussion naturally help students retain information because when students see their fellow classmates, that visual will spark memories of what those students had to say about the lesson in question. In short, participatory methods of teaching and learning should be used in the class during teaching and learning process.
5.Include constructive comments on graded assignment or tests
Each comment becomes another synaptic connection enforcing the “memory web” for a given concept or fact.
6.Encourage students to make lesson review the last thing they do before bedtime or rehearsing
Research shows the information circulates in the mind during sleep, bolstering retention.
7.Employ the spacing effect
Research shows that revisiting lessons just as they are on the verge of being forgotten boosts memory retention. Instead of teaching one lesson, considering it complete, then moving on to the next lesson, a better approach is to teach a lesson but then revisit it at regular intervals in the future.

Thank you!

Prepared by;
Mwl. Juma,
Mwenge Catholic University
Department of Education, Educational Psychology & Guidance and Counselling



No comments

Theme images by jangeltun. Powered by Blogger.